New Teachers Learning Disciplined Improvisation for Meaningful
Talk in Diverse Classrooms
Conceptions of Discussion, Teaching, Teacher Learning
We clarify conceptions. By discussion, we mean group
structures for recall, language production/elaboration, exploring
interpretations, linking content and community, and working
through problems and conflicting ideas. Teaching through
discussion includes substantive engagement associated with
academic learning, using authentic questions, uptake for cohesive
discourse, and high-quality feedback (Applebee, Langer, Nystrand,
& Gamoran, 2003; Nystrand, 1997). Such discussion facilitates
discovery and display through language (Bunch, 2013); scaffolding
language of schooling (Schleppegrell, 2004); and leveraging
linguistic resources in students’ communicative repertoires
(Martinez, Morales, & Aldana, 2017). Such teaching includes
disciplined improvisation with moments co-constructed in
real time. By disciplined we refer to content purposes
for discussion, repertoires of knowledge and techniques
(Erickson, 2011), repertoires of practice (Gutierrez & Rogoff,
2003), structures and constraints (Hammerness et al., 2005;
Sawyer, 2004). By improvisation we mean in-the-moment
decision-making and microadaptations (Barker, 2012; Corno, 2008)
to focus engagement and learning.
Relatively little is known about how teachers learn to
enact discussion: conceptual and practical tools needed
(Williamson, 2013), adaptations teachers learn to make, a
typology of moves available. A slim literature finds
microadaptations in literacy lessons include: modifying
objectives, changing means by which objectives are met (adapt
routines, strategies, materials, actions), inventing examples or
analogies, inserting mini-lessons, suggesting different ways
students can handle a problem, and resequencing or omitting
activities (Duffy et al., 2008; Parsons, 2012). Adaptations
require keen perception, and issues warranting adaptation may go
unnoticed, especially by novices who may not see “telltale signs
for adjustment” (Duffy et al., 2009, p. 168). Pattern recognition
requires development, as it is seldom a natural process for
teachers (Korthagen, 2010). Challenges of facilitating oral
language activity intensify for many new teachers underprepared
to support learners in linguistically complex classrooms (Ball,
2009; Darling-Hammond, 2006; Gándara, Maxwell-Jolly, & Driscoll,
2005), such as those we feature in our project.
As classroom talk may be fast-paced and ephemeral (Wortham,
2008), adaptation in-the-moment is demanding. Teacher development
that supports perceptual skills in noticing language
contributions, nonverbal cues, and efforts at discourse, and
metacognitive processing of action and talk may support
teachers’ capacity to reflect-in-action (Schon, 1983; Yanow &
Tsoukas, 2009) and make informed choices that support learning.
We need to know much more about adaptations a teacher might make
and ways teachers learn to enact them.
Cognition and Teacher Learning for Discussion
Our project investigates cognitive underpinnings of disciplined
improvisation, a form of adaptive expertise (Bransford et al.,
2005) that benefits from conceptually rich knowledge of teaching
practices and their effects. Like other expert decision-making,
disciplined improvisation involves knowledge-driven and
perceptual-driven processes (also called top-down and bottom-up
processing; Chi, Feltovich, & Glaser, 1981). For example,
teachers must foster discourse incorporating all voices (e.g.,
noticing potentially relevant observations voiced outside of
“standard” academic language). A number of studies explore
teacher noticing (Sherin, Jacobs, & Phillip, 2011), but
perceptual aspects of disciplined improvisation have received
little attention.
Instruction focused exclusively on building repertoires of
knowledge typically fails to influence practice (Bransford et
al., 2005). This problem of enactment arises from a
paucity of opportunity to rehearse these learned strategies. Lack
of focus on perceptual processing likely exacerbates the problem,
as teachers may fail to notice conditions of applicability. We
will develop both a process model (how does disciplinary
improvisation work in-the-moment) and a learning model
(how do teachers learn to enact disciplined improvisation) to
support classroom discourse in linguistically diverse classrooms.
There remains little empirical elaboration of underlying
processes of disciplined improvisation. Sawyer (2011) argues that
teaching expertise is “mastery of a corpus of
knowledge—ready-made, known solutions to standard problems–but
in a special way that supports improvisational practice.” In our
simplified process model (Figure 1), disciplined improvisation is
informed by knowledge and practice, informed by
perceptual learning of meaningful patterns, and
ultimately responsive in-the-moment—through
microadaptations—to diverse students’ learning, language, and
lives. Metacognition—awareness of one’s own knowledge and skill,
and ability to control one’s cognition—knits these pieces
together. Our research will elucidate repertoires of knowledge
and practice and perceptual skills—and relationships between
them—necessary to support discourse in linguistically diverse
classrooms.
Our initial model of how disciplined improvisation can be learned
(Figure 2) is also highly simplified: teachers need to develop
their repertoires of knowledge and
practice, they need to develop
perceptual skills, and they need ample
opportunities to develop the metacognitive
skills needed to integrate knowledge-driven and
perception-driven processing in authentic contexts. Our
definition of teacher learning is growth in any of these
three areas. With teacher partners, we will design
educational experiences for early-career teachers so we can
observe, analyze, and improve designs for teacher learning across
these three areas.
Refined Research Questions
Our overarching question is:
How do teachers learn to enact disciplined improvisation
in support of discourse in linguistically diverse
classrooms?
We examine this overarching question through three main
sub-questions. For each, we look at the nature of the expertise
(process model) and how it is learned through experience
(learning model):
RQ1. Learning the dialogic toolkit
- What knowledge and practices support
disciplined improvisation in linguistically diverse classrooms?
- What learning activities and tools support their development?
RQ2. Learning to see in discourse
- What perceptual/noticing skills support
disciplined improvisation in linguistically diverse classrooms?
- What learning activities and tools support their development?
RQ3. Enacting disciplined improvisation in the moment
- What metacognitive skills support the
integration of knowledge, practice, and perception into
disciplined improvisation?
- What learning activities and tools support their development?
Alignment with Program Goals and Response to Feedback
Early-career teacher learning. We have found
even when preservice teachers (PSTs) develop rich repertoires of
knowledge and practice for discussion through coursework, first
enactments are fraught with complexity, needing greater retrieval
of coursework information and knowledge synthesis. TE needs
innovative pedagogical models to develop agentive educators and
engage teachers in research-informed practice, particularly in
light of “reforms” stripping TE of its research-based and
visionary roles (Zeichner, 2014). As part of a McDonnell-funded
collective, we will add focus to preservice/early-career teachers
and models of their learning and of TE pedagogy to support
development of novices for communicative work in diverse
classrooms.
ELA, then science. Responding to feedback, we
examined ways our design-based research with TE and teacher
partners has yielded rich extant databases in ELA that inform
study plans. Emergent findings will inform focus on elementary
science, so we can test our models, aligned with the program call
for “cross-cutting impact across subject areas and ages.”
Scale-down. We focus now on nuanced examination
of teacher learning and model development in California’s richly
diverse population, eliminating the UMD bilingual teachers team.
We also no longer will study dissemination with CSU, instead
tapping CSU faculty as advisors and going deep with preservice
and early-career teachers in local schools.
Virtual reality (VR). Feedback raised VR
concerns: transferability, “authenticity” versus live
improvisation, cost/sustainability. We recognize concerns but
find VR a promising, rapidly-evolving avenue with affordances
that can be leveraged, combined with face-to-face teacher
learning supports (teacher educator feedback, professional
learning communities). Opportunity for iterative change over time
in a lower-risk environment (real students not impacted as
teachers practice) is a significant VR affordance, as is creation
of tailored, reusable learning environments ensuring PSTs sustain
practice with important discussion aspects to which they may have
uneven exposure in real-world settings (e.g., facilitating
discussion in linguistically diverse classrooms, distinguishing
uptake/pseudo-uptake).
Another key VR affordance is ability to record authentic
interactions to return to and iterate upon, making ephemeral
discussion a durable artifact for analysis and improvement. When
people interact with human-controlled avatars versus
algorithm-controlled agents, their physiological reactions and
behaviors are similar to how they would react to real people
(e.g., Donath, 2007; Hoyt, Blascovich, & Swinth, 2003; Okita,
Bailenson, & Schwartz, 2008). This suggests that immersive
virtual environments with avatars that we would use may provide
novel, contextualized classroom talk situations that are
realistic, potentially augmenting face-to-face preparation by
repeatedly drawing teachers’ attention to consequential elements
of classroom discussion that can go unseen or unexplored in
real-world settings.
We are aware that VR study reviews reveal uneven results (e.g.,
Khan et al., 2009; Laver et al., 2015), and there is need for
rigorous research that explicitly addresses transfer from virtual
to real-world settings. However, we are encouraged by some
investigations suggesting cognitive strategy elements of these
two environments are broadly equivalent and that there is a clear
positive transfer effect from virtual to real training when
virtual task elements closely approximate real-world task
elements (e.g., Rose et al., 1998, 2000). We will cautiously
build on this promising research by learning about teachers’
local contexts and histories in classroom discussion practices,
then designing virtual environments that reflect realities of
those contexts. Additionally, our approach to transfer differs
from many reviewed studies (particularly in rehabilitation) which
feature spontaneous transfer. We will support teachers in guided
transfer through activities designed to bridge learning between
virtual and real-classroom environments. Finally, we intend to
contribute to literature on VR for training with systematic
reporting of study details and investment in understanding
teachers’ learning and development across virtual and
face-to-face settings over time.